NC
Note: **: p < 0.01; *: p < 0.05. NC = normal condition; SC = articulatory suppression condition; and Vinegrad_GC = Vinegrad general characteristics sub_scale.
Prematching descriptive statistics and results of the independent sample t -test.
Group | Levene’s Test for Equality of Variances | Independent Sample -Test | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
5th Percentile ( = 48) | LD ( = 51) | F | df | adj-p | d | ||||
Text-Dictation-Errors | 8.31 (4.52) | 11.24 (8.01) | 3.15 | <0.001 | 2.25 | 79.79 | 0.027 | 0.039 | 0.50 [0.10; 0.90] |
Words-Errors-NC | 5.58 (4.33) | 6.84 (6.52) | 2.26 | 0.005 | 1.14 | 87.46 | 0.258 | 0.286 | 0.24 [−0.15; 0.64] |
Words-Errors-SC | 13.69 (8.11) | 21.25 (12.89) | 2.53 | 0.002 | 3.52 | 84.92 | 0.001 | 0.002 | 0.76 [0.35; 1.17] |
Graphemes-NC | 130.75 (38.48) | 130.86 (35.31) | 0.84 | 0.549 | 0.02 | 97.00 | 0.988 | 0.988 | 0.00 [−0.39; 0.40] |
Graphemes-ErrorsNC | 1.31 (1.65) | 2.29 (3.35) | 4.12 | <0.001 | 1.86 | 73.88 | 0.066 | 0.083 | 0.43 [0.03; 0.83] |
Graphemes-SC | 113.46 (37.42) | 95.12 (36.41) | 0.95 | 0.848 | −2.47 | 97.00 | 0.015 | 0.025 | 0.50 [0.10; 0.90] |
Graphemes-ErrorsSC | 2.00 (2.44) | 3.52 (3.35) | 1.88 | 0.031 | 2.59 | 91.42 | 0.011 | 0.023 | 0.54 [0.14; 0.94] |
Vinegrad-Total | 7.10 (4.24) | 15.00 (5.41) | 1.63 | 0.093 | 8.05 | 97.00 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 1.64 [1.18; 2.09] |
Vinegrad-GC | 1.98 (1.44) | 4.06 (2.01) | 1.97 | 0.021 | 5.95 | 90.50 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 1.25 [0.82; 1.68] |
Vinegrad-Writing | 1.73 (1.61) | 3.78 (1.63) | 1.03 | 0.932 | 6.31 | 97.00 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 1.28 [0.85; 1.71] |
Note: df: degrees of freedom; p : raw p -value; adj- p : false-discovery-rate adjusted p -value [ 43 ]; and d: Cohen’s d, with its 95% confidence interval. NC = normal condition; SC = articulatory suppression condition; and Vinegrad_GC = Vinegrad general characteristics sub_scale.
To investigate which differences may exist in terms of the number of errors and speed in subgroups 1 and 2 (see the descriptive statistics reported in Table 2 ), after testing the association pattern between the variables, groups were compared using independent sample t -tests to investigate which tasks were more challenging for the LD group. We then used propensity score analysis (PSA [ 39 ]) to investigate the net group differences in self-perceived difficulties when matched with the writing tasks performance. PSA has been recommended as a more principled method than analysis of covariance to account for the imbalance of groups regarding relevant covariates, as the latter method is likely to provide biased and inconsistent estimates of group differences if not all relevant interactions and nonlinear effects are included in the model [ 40 ]. PSA was performed with the matchit function in the MatchIt [ 41 ] package in R using the genetic method [ 42 ].
Descriptive statistics and correlations are reported in Table 1 and Table 2 . The writing tasks showed a pattern of significant correlations that supported the association among writing tasks, consistent with the pattern reported in the manual of the battery [ 37 ]. Significant correlations were also found within self-report measures and between writing tasks and self-report measures (see Table 1 ).
The results of the independent sample t -tests showed that there were large differences (Cohen’s d > 0.80) in self-perceived difficulties between the 5th percentile and the LD group, but this result could be an artefact of small-to-large differences in writing task scores ( Table 2 ). The two groups significantly differed in the articulatory suppression condition tasks, both in terms of the number of graphemes (a variable that measures slowness vs. fastness in writing) and accuracy (number of errors committed in both the word dictation task and the writing-numbers-in-words task), but not in the standard condition tasks, with the only exception of the dictation task.
When we performed PSA to test net group differences in self-perceived difficulties, we found a large difference in pre-matching distance scores (i.e., the logit-transformed probability of a case of belonging to its actual group given the covariates—in this case, writing tasks scores) (LD group: 0.39 ± 0.23; 5th percentile group: 0.59 ± 0.17; t (87.85) = 4.96, p < 0.001, d = 0.99 [0.56; 1.41]).
The matching procedure discarded 31 observations in the LD group that could not contribute to achieving a balance between the groups for the writing task scores. Hence, the final analyses were performed for the original 5th percentile group participants ( n = 48), who were considered the reference group and thus assigned a weight of 1 by the procedure, and 21 participants from the LD group, who were considered the focal group and were assigned a weight ranging from 0.42 to 3.33 (median = 0.62) to reach an adequate balance. After matching, the difference in distance scores was no longer significant (LD group: 0.55 ± 0.16; 5th percentile group: 0.59 ± 0.17; t (67) = 0.20, p = 0.845, d = 0.05 [−0.46; 0.57]), and the groups were adequately balanced in terms of the writing task scores ( Table 3 ). Nevertheless, the first three rows of Table 3 show that even after matching, the group differences in the self-perceived difficulties were still large, suggesting that they could not be accounted for by differences in the writing task performance.
Postmatching descriptive statistics and results of the independent sample t -test.
Group | Levene’s Test for Equality of Variances | Independent Sample -Test | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
5th Percentile ( = 48) | LD ( = 20) | F | df | adj-p | d | ||||
Text-Dictation-Errors | 8.31 (4.52) | 8.45 (4.45) | 1.02 | 0.458 | 0.45 | 66.00 | 0.809 | 0.991 | 0.12 [−0.40; 0.64] |
Words-Errors-NC | 5.58 (4.33) | 6.60 (5.38) | 1.62 | 0.089 | 0.77 | 66.00 | 0.668 | 0.991 | 0.21 [−0.32; 0.73] |
Words-Errors-SC | 13.69 (8.11) | 15.09 (6.86) | 0.75 | 0.212 | 0.02 | 66.00 | 0.991 | 0.991 | 0.01 [−0.52; 0.53] |
Graphemes-NC | 130.75 (38.48) | 134.15 (25.83) | 0.47 | 0.019 | 0.43 | 51.82 | 0.672 | 0.991 | 0.11 [−0.41; 0.64] |
Graphemes-ErrorsNC | 1.31 (1.65) | 1.30 (1.36) | 0.71 | 0.170 | 0.20 | 66.00 | 0.919 | 0.991 | 0.05 [−0.47; 0.57] |
Graphemes-SC | 113.46 (37.42) | 110.25 (28.25) | 0.60 | 0.079 | 0.06 | 66.00 | 0.976 | 0.991 | 0.02 [−0.51; 0.54] |
Graphemes-ErrorsSC | 2.00 (2.44) | 2.30 (1.87) | 0.61 | 0.089 | 0.03 | 66.00 | 0.986 | 0.991 | 0.01 [−0.51; 0.53] |
Vinegrad-Total | 7.10 (4.24) | 14.20 (5.38) | 1.70 | 0.071 | 6.71 | 66.00 | <0.001 | 0.001 | 1.81 [1.20; 2.41] |
Vinegrad-GC | 1.98 (1.44) | 3.85 (1.81) | 1.67 | 0.077 | 5.29 | 66.00 | 0.001 | 0.006 | 1.43 [0.85; 2.00] |
Vinegrad-Writing | 1.73 (1.61) | 3.45 (1.66) | 1.13 | 0.358 | 5.06 | 66.00 | 0.002 | 0.006 | 1.37 [0.79; 1.93] |
Note: df: degrees of freedom; p : raw p -value; adj-p: false-discovery-rate adjusted p -value [ 43 ]; and d: Cohen’s d, with its 95% confidence interval. NC = normal condition; SC = articulatory suppression condition; and Vinegrad_gen_char = Vinegrad general characteristics sub_scale.
This study compared the performance of inaccurate writers who were not diagnosed with LDs with that of students who were diagnosed with LDs in an attempt to identify which tasks are the most challenging for individuals with LDs, while also investigating whether inaccurate writers with and without a diagnosis differ in terms of self-perceived difficulties. An innovative contribution of the present study is that it analysed the difficulties experienced by individuals with LDs that may last until young adulthood using a sample of inaccurate writers, performing below the 5th percentile in the writing tasks, as a control group. Moreover, we studied the impact that being diagnosed with LDs may have on the self-perception of difficulty, particularly with regard to writing skills. The results regarding writing tasks showed that the two groups significantly differed in the articulatory suppression condition tasks, but not in the standard condition tasks, with the only exception of the text dictation task. Furthermore, with regard to the perception of difficulty, students who were diagnosed with LDs reported significantly more perceived difficulties, pertaining to the writing tasks selected for the purpose of this research, than inaccurate peers. Students with LDs also reported significantly more perceived difficulties after the two groups were matched for performance on writing tasks.
The correlation analyses confirmed a homogenous pattern of association among writing tasks and between writing tasks and perceived difficulties assessed by the general and writing-related items and by the total score of the Vinegrad [ 44 ]. Consistent with the literature, the comparisons of the two groups only outlined significant differences in the most challenging tasks, namely, all the tasks administered in the articulatory suppression condition and, to a lesser extent, in the text dictation task in a normal condition. Although university students with LDs are presumably able to compensate for most of their difficulties when reaching higher education and their performance in many tasks can be comparable to that of students belonging to the extreme of the normal distribution, specific characteristics of individuals with LDs emerge when they face tasks that involve a higher cognitive load. In simple tasks such as word dictation and grapheme writing, their performance does not differ from that of inaccurate writers, whereas they encounter particular problems achieving this level of performance in more difficult tasks. The condition of articulatory suppression interferes with the activation of the word subvocal rehearsal strategy during transcription, which usually allows continuous retrieval of the composition of the word. Generally, students diagnosed with dyslexia and dysorthographia compensate by articulating words during transcription, and whenever they are unable to apply this strategy, their performance tends to decline. An inaccurate writer performance, conversely, seems not to be particularly challenged by this condition, in terms of mean scores, possibly because, globally, they managed to establish better access to coding. The rehearsal process implied in the articulatory suppression condition seems to interfere with the normal functioning of working memory: Students who are able to fully automate the writing process and orthographic rules and rely on the lexical representation of the words are not particularly affected by this interference effect. Literature reporting results on adults with dyslexia, on the contrary, documents that dyslexic adults are strongly influenced by this specific interference effect [ 45 ]. From this perspective, students with LDs may need to rely on the sublexical route (accessed via subvocal rehearsal) not having immediate access to intact phonetic representations [ 17 ], which would also support a rapid retrieval of word representations while writing. Overall, considering the same tasks when performed under normal conditions, our results show that students who were diagnosed with LDs did not differ with respect to inaccurate writers.
The text dictation task, in which the two groups showed significant differences, represents an exception to this trend in terms of accuracy. This result may be due to the difficulties that writing under dictation for a more prolonged interval of time may present, in terms of the rhythm that needs to be followed and time, which may not allow the use of the subvocal rehearsal, especially when words are long, infrequent, and dictated continuously. Moreover, during the text dictation task, the students need to follow the experimenter’s dictation rhythm by writing at the same pace, and the perceived need to be fast (although the experimenter was instructed to adapt, to some extent, to the rhythm of the participant) might interfere with the LDs participants’ performance and result in a more inaccurate outcome. Interestingly, when considering standard tasks, such as word dictation and grapheme production, students with LDs did not seem to make more errors than students who were within the tails of the normal distribution. The tasks performed under the articulatory suppression condition cannot be classified as standard everyday tasks that were familiar to the student, but were specifically created to discriminate the specific characteristics of LDs [ 22 ].
The differences between the two groups registered in the Vinegrad+ questionnaire are particularly interesting. This self-report measure specifically focuses on the perception of difficulty, and the results clearly indicate that participants who were already diagnosed with LDs tended to report significantly more perceived difficulties compared to inaccurate writers who had never been evaluated for or diagnosed with LDs.
The analysis performed on the matched samples allowed us to test whether the differences found between the perceptions of the two groups were linked to the actual differences found in the performance of specific tasks that we described above. The results confirmed that even when balancing the performance of the two groups for all tasks under the articulatory suppression condition, which emerged as the most discriminative for students with a diagnosis of LDs, participants with LDs reported significantly more perceived difficulties than students identified as inaccurate writers—who, in some cases, possibly had an LD that had not yet been detected. This result, in particular, may be of specific interest in discussing the all-around implications of being diagnosed with LDs and the need to work not only on the learning cognitive mechanism, but also on more general aspects, such as self-esteem, self-efficacy, and self-worth. The literature indicates how students with LDs benefit more from specific activities, such as first-year preparation courses, before entering university [ 31 ], as they show greater improvement in terms of academic self-efficacy compared to their typical peers. In general, these results highlight the importance of analysing all the emotional aspects that may be connected to and triggered by a diagnosis of an LD in depth. Being diagnosed with an LD can increase students’ awareness of their own difficulties, with possible positive effects on the use of effective learning and metacognitive strategies [ 46 , 47 ]. Nevertheless, it is important not to underestimate the possible negative impact of the diagnosis on self-efficacy and self-esteem, which must be taken into account when guiding students at any level of schooling.
The emerged enhanced perception of difficulty is a finding consistent with the limited existing literature on the topic. The results from a comprehensive study on the efficacy of university preparation courses by Reed et al. [ 31 ] outlined that students with learning disabilities in college report lower levels of self-efficacy beliefs than their non-LD peers. Reed et al. [ 31 ] highlighted that students with learning disabilities in higher education tend to report less confidence in their capabilities to meet academic demands, question their overall academic competencies, and show increased pessimistic attitudes towards completing higher education requirements. Lower levels of academic self-efficacy beliefs are argued by researchers to translate into a diminished sense of capacity for learning challenging academic curricula [ 10 ]. Consistent with the limited existing literature on the topic, these results support the idea that individuals with identified LDs are significantly more likely to encounter challenges in maintaining a good level of motivation and persistence in trying to overcome the difficulties they face, on a daily basis, due to lower levels of self-efficacy beliefs. In the long run, unlike peers with LDs who express lower levels of self-efficacy beliefs, individuals with LDs who have positive and accurate self-efficacy beliefs are more likely to achieve independence and autonomy within postsecondary learning environments [ 11 ].
The results presented in the current study show that the enhanced perception of difficulty concerns not only specific difficulties related to writing skills, but also general learning features (considering everyday activities that imply reading and writing processes, e.g., consulting a map, and one’s abilities relative to linguistic components that can be automated) and the overall learning experience assessed by the Vinegrad+ total score. These results suggest important areas for intervention that not only take into account the cognitive features and challenges that individuals with LDs face, but also the emotional impact of a diagnosis on the general perception of ability that, in turn, may influence future outcomes, such as career choice and the drop-out rate in higher levels of education [ 48 ]. This attitude towards education and learning, in fact, may negatively influence one’s career path choice, directing individuals towards something that they perceive as easier and/or less demanding, instead of something that is truly of interest to them and that they feel passionate about, with a subsequent impact on their future work life and, possibly, an enduring sense of general frustration and low-self efficacy beliefs. From this perspective, working on this emotional facet of the condition would not only magnify the effectiveness of the cognitive training on the implementation of strategies, but would also result in a protective factor that could reasonably reduce feelings, such as anxiety, and a perceived lack of efficacy, increasing the ability to tolerate fatigue and frustration; working on this facet of the condition would also preclude the creation of negative emotional anticipation associated with learning and education.
In conclusion, the results of the present study are consistent with previous findings on the compensation that university students with LDs are able to implement, on one hand, and on the tendency of these students to internalize an experience of difficulty and fatigue with regard to learning, on the other hand [ 30 , 31 ].
Some limitations of this study warrant mentioning. First, as students with LDs are relatively rare in Italian universities, the sample size was inevitably not very large, and this might have impacted the statistical power. Future research in this domain should attempt to replicate these results using larger samples. Second, we only used one self-report measure for perceived difficulties: The use of multiple measures and the collection of data from observers, such as peers, teachers, tutors, and/or parents, will help to provide a more comprehensive assessment of the phenomenon. Despite these limitations, this study adds knowledge to a relatively less investigated stage of development and the features of LDs in young adulthood. In addition, it provides evidence of differences in the individuals’ perception of difficulties while controlling for performance, suggesting important areas for intervention. From this perspective, these results address the crucial role that receiving a diagnosis has in guaranteeing that students with LDs have access to all the instruments and tutelage that can allow them to proficiently experience learning at university. Moreover, the results provided by the present study highlight the importance of investing in practices that support students emotionally, with a specific focus on self-efficacy and self-esteem beliefs, which, together with the above mentioned tutelages and instruments, constitute a core protective factor for preventing drop-out, allowing students to confidently persevere in their careers. An important advancement in this regard would be the implementation of a specific support service at university to work with LD students on both the emotional impact of diagnosis and specific strategies and methodologies for supporting them in studying. These practices would increase and support student’s self-efficacy and self-esteem, working on both performance and academic “identity” at the same time.
Overall, in this study, we showed how students with LDs and writing difficulties can reach a certain level of efficient compensation. Nevertheless, students with LDs perceived significantly more difficulties with respect to their undiagnosed peers. This result was also confirmed when matching the two groups in terms of writing task scores, suggesting the importance of addressing self-perception and self-efficacy issues in these students.
M.Z. and L.B., conceptualization; C.M., M.Z., and. L.B., participated in the investigation for the standardization of the battery used; C.M., participated in the data curation; C.C., ran the analysis; C.M., writing—original draft preparation; C.M., M.Z., C.C., and L.B.; writing—review and editing, M.Z. and L.B., supervision. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
This research received no external funding.
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of the University of Genoa (official communication received on 20 March 2017).
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Conflicts of interest.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser .
Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.
2021, IOER International Multidisciplinary Research Journal
Using a descriptive-correlational research design, this study is an attempt to determine the relationship between the students’ perception on research writing difficulties and their performance in Practical Research. Specifically, it dealt on finding the perceived writing difficulties of 80 senior high school STEM and HUMSS students with regard to the technical aspect and process of research writing through a closedended survey questionnaire; evaluating the performance of students in their actual research outputs through researcher-modified holistic and analytic rubrics; and finding the correlation between the students’ perceived writing difficulties and their performance in Practical Research using Pearson ProductMoment Correlation Coefficient. The results indicate that the respondents generally agree to have been experiencing difficulties in both the technical aspect that includes research paper format and grammar and sentence construction; as well as in the process of research writing that includes steps in the research process and working on the different parts of the research paper. In addition, the results of the research outputs’ evaluation reveal that the students manifest developing skills in their performance in Practical Research. The findings of the study entail that difficulties in both the technical aspect and process of research writing confront the students in the senior high school. While the negative correlation coefficient results indicate that most of the students who have high perceived writing difficulties have low performance in research, the findings show that there is no statistically significant evidence that perceived writing difficulties and performance in Practical Research are correlated. In the light of the findings, the study recommends the need for the assessment of students’ difficulties in research writing. Identifying these difficulties can help the Practical Research teachers gauge their students' readiness for academic writing skills, which can have a big impact on the quality of their research outputs. Additionally, more activities and exercises may be provided by research teachers to help students improve their performance in Practical Research, particularly in the areas of content and focus, sources and format, and conventions. These activities could take the form of courses and modules that focus on note-taking, summarizing, and paraphrasing, all of which are critical skills in research writing. More so, the number of research teacher training programs may be increased to help teachers deal with students' research writing problems and other research-writing concerns.
Sri Widiastuti
IOSR Journals
jennifer de ramos , Jennifer Tan
PEOPLE: International Journal of Social Sciences
Precy Paurillo
International Journal of Research Publications
Russel Aporbo
Rima Juniar
Studies in Linguistics and Literature
Amir Hossain
Journal of English Language Teaching
Rusdi Noor Rosa
Jurnal Pemberdayaan Masyarakat Madani (JPMM)
Nuramalia Hasanah
Vinicius M. Netto
GEMMALINE BUMANGLAG
Marcia Farr
ELS Journal on Interdisciplinary Studies in Humanities
Dian Agustina Purwanto Wakerkwa
AJHSSR Journal
IAA Journal of Applied Sciences
EZE V A L H Y G I N U S UDOKA
Renaldi Bimantoro
The Scientific Journal of Cihan University – Sulaimaniya
Barham S Abdulrahman
Jurnal Iqra' : Kajian Ilmu Pendidikan
Lusy T Muharlisiani
Su-Hie Ting
Dr. Ajare oloruntoba
Acitya : Journal of Teaching and Education
Yanti Sri Rezeki
JPBI (Jurnal Pendidikan Biologi Indonesia)
Chaidir Adam
International Journal of Linguistics
khalid al-zuoud
Monika Sobejko
Background sources.
Welcome to the research guide for 76-108 Writing about Public Problems (WaPP). This guide will introduce you to research tools and strategies you can use to craft persuasive, public facing arguments.
On the Find Sources page here, learn how to:
On the Subject-Specific Databases page , learn how to:
On the Access Sources page , learn how to:
On the Evaluate Sources page, learn how to:
On the Citations page , learn how to:
And on the Need Help? page, learn where you can go for help with research, writing, and other needs.
Companion sources, Handbooks, and specialized encyclopedias are great places to start. Most are available online through the Libraries Catalog.
IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
Difficulty in distinguishing between content and structure. ... honed, and applied to the goal of writing and publishing quality scientific research. Writing to learn is a novel and evolving concept with diverse and fragmented views . In accord with this concept, informal writing as a way of enhancing personal understanding has numerous ...
Specifically, it dealt on finding the perceived writing difficulties of 80 senior high school STEM and HUMSS students with regard to the technical aspect and process of research writing through a closedended survey questionnaire; evaluating the performance of students in their actual research outputs through researcher-modified holistic and ...
Abstract and Figures. This study attempted to investigate the students' difficulties in research writing involving 39 master's students majoring in English Language Education at one of the ...
In the qualitative part, we identified three main themes in analyzing the interviews that showed students' problems in writing the literature review section as follows: 5.1. Lack of knowledge of writing effective literature review. Most interviewees (80%) were not aware of the importance of the literature review.
Writing qualitative research is a complex activity. Yet there is relatively little research about novices' experiences in learning to write this genre. The purpose of this multiple case study is to explore the challenges students face when they first encounter the qualitative research paradigm. Drawing upon interviews with students, think-aloud ...
4. Not enriching your paper with appropriate figures and tables. Using text to describe everything can be boring and might not help you put your results/data across as effectively as a table or a figure. Adding figures and tables also help you flesh out your results section and give it the emphasis it deserves.
Research has shown that writing process is related directly with the doctoral students' identity development and is not just can be seen as a skill acquisition but a socio-cultural tool that need to be learnt. ... The highest rated areas of difficulty were the writing process (25%), followed by developing content/ideas (24%), use of grammar ...
In other words, writing is hard for everyone. In what follows, I detail some of the struggles of my early scientific writing experience while offering valuable lessons that I found helpful. Everyone Struggles. Writing a research manuscript is difficult on many levels. The structure of a scientific manuscript differs from undergraduate writing ...
Three research instruments were utilized: a 30-item questionnaire investigating participants' perceptions of writing problems, a pre- and post-writing test assessing students' writing ...
Academic writing is a skill essential for anyone looking to pursue higher academics. Academic writing is a specific type of writing that involves a lot of reading or material collection, doing in-depth research and critical analysis of scholarly literature, planning of the entire presentation, revising contents and structure, etc. Invariably it ends up involving rewriting, editing ...
Let's be perfectly honest: Writing an academic paper can be a real slog and it often takes weeks, if not months—and sometimes years. For most of us, writing occurs in several stages and includes a lot of revisions. Roald Dahl once said, "Good writing is essentially rewriting." What is true of children's books is true of scientific writing, too.
Purpose: Writing scientific articles is a daunting task for novice researchers. In this qualitative study carried out in 2007, the authors evaluated the experiences of a group of novice researchers engaged in the writing process, to elucidate the main difficulties and sources of encouragement they encountered. Method: Sixteen novice researchers ...
Take your time with the planning process. "It's worth consulting other researchers, doing a pilot study to test it, before you go out spending the time, money, and energy to do the big study," Crawford says. "Because once you begin the study, you can't stop.". Challenge: Assembling a Research Team.
Researchers will have a better understanding of the following: Importance of good English writing skills. Improved knowledge about the application of English grammar in academic writing. Importance of fluency, clarity, and readability of the manuscript. Awareness of common mistakes made by ESL researchers while writing in English.
8.Understanding and following the nuances of academic and scientific ethics. Research ethics are among the top challenges faced by researchers. Plagiarism, falsification, fabrication, missing ethical declarations and non-compliance with standard ethical guidelines are considered inappropriate and can have serious consequences for researchers.
The study showed that the cooperation of respondents. outside the ac ademic insti tution is very crucial problem for student researchers. To. some extent, students felt that personal problems like ...
claimed that the majority of the students have encountered serious problems in writing a good English essay (Personal information, October 24th, 2019). Contemporary researches on essay writing difficulties have been mushrooming in many different settings. However, a study about investigating essay writing difficulties encountered by tertiary
The study explored various and common challenges/difficulties during writing the research proposals and projects such as: difficulty in deciding the topic for research, lack of good knowledge of the methodology, inability of finding modern, specialized and related references, lack of interest in research, lack of understanding of the subject ...
1) of students and established researchers learning to conduct and write qualitative research. Their research was concerned with the interplay between affect and cognition: how people feel and what they learn. An important study by Li and Searle (2007) explored students' experiences of learning to do qualitative data analysis.
When we consider difficulties in writing acquisition, it is helpful to think about word, sentence, and text levels (Fayol et al., 2012; Hayes & Berninger, 2014).This has several important advantages in capturing writing difficulties; it ensures that the researcher and practitioner link writing to the levels of language necessary to produce a proficient text and as such can capture bottlenecks ...
Research has shown that academic success is strongly associated with positive academic self-efficacy beliefs and that individuals with learning disabilities (LDs) usually report a lower perception of competence than their peers in most learning domains. ... Overall, in this study, we showed how students with LDs and writing difficulties can ...
between the students' perception on research writing difficulties and their performance in Practical Research. Specifically, it dealt on finding the perceived writing difficulties of 80 senior high school STEM and HUMSS students with regard to the technical aspect and process of research writing through a closed-
Specifically, it dealt on finding the perceived writing difficulties of 80 senior high school STEM and HUMSS students with regard to the technical aspect and process of research writing through a closed ended survey questionnaire; evaluating the performance of students in their actual research outputs through researcher-modified holistic and ...
Welcome to the research guide for 76-108 Writing about Public Problems (WaPP). This guide will introduce you to research tools and strategies you can use to craft persuasive, public facing arguments. On the Find Sources page here, learn how to: Search the Libraries Catalog; Find & search library databases